09-robbe.pdf

Cannabis and Car Driving
Introduction
This article describes the results of a research program that was set up to determine thedose-response relationship between marijuana and objectively and subjectivelymeasured aspects of real world driving; and to determine whether it is possible tocorrelate driving performance impairment with plasma concentrations of the drug or ametabolite. The program consisted of three driving studies in which a variety of drivingtasks were employed, including: maintenance of a constant speed and lateral positionduring uninterrupted highway travel, following a leading car with varying speed on ahighway, and city driving. A laboratory study preceded the driving studies for identifyingthe highest THC dose to be administered in the subsequent studies.
General Procedures
Subjects in all studies were recreational users of marijuana or hashish, i.e., smoking thedrug more than once a month, but not daily. They were all healthy, between 21 and 40years of age, had normal weight and binocular acuity, and were licensed to drive anautomobile. Furthermore, law enforcement authorities were contacted, with thevolunteers' consent, to verify that they had no previous arrests or convictions for drunkendriving or drug trafficking.
Each subject was required to submit a urine sample immediately upon arrival at the test site. Samples were assayed qualitatively for the following common `street drugs' (ormetabolites): cannabinoids, benzodiazepines, opiates, cocaine, amphetamines andbarbiturates. In addition a breath sample was analyzed for the presence of alcohol.
Blood samples were repeatedly taken after smoking by venepuncture. Quantitativeanalysis of THC and THC-COOH in plasma was performed by gas chromatography/massspectrometry (GC/MS) using deuterated cannabinoids as internal standards.
Marijuana and placebo marijuana cigarettes were supplied by the U.S. National Instituteon Drug Abuse. The lowest and highest THC concentrations in the marijuana cigarettesused in the studies were 1.75% and 3.57%, respectively. Subjects smoked theadministered cigarettes through a plastic holder in their customary fashion.
Subjects were accompanied during every driving test by a licensed driving instructor.
A redundant control system in the test vehicle was available for controlling the car,should emergency situations arise.
In each study, subjects repeatedly performed certain simple laboratory tests (e.g.
critical instability tracking, hand and posture stability), estimated their levels ofintoxication and indicated their willingness to drive under several specified conditions ofurgency. In addition, heart rate and blood pressure were measured. Results of thesemeasurements are reported elsewhere (Robbe, 1994).
Laboratory Study
Methods
Twenty-four subjects, equally comprised of men and women, participated in this study.
They were allowed to smoke part or all of the THC content in three cigarettes until
achieving the desired psychological effect. The only requirement was to smoke for a
period not exceeding 15 minutes. When subjects voluntarily stopped smoking, cigarettes
were carefully extinguished and retained for subsequent gravimetric estimation of the
amount of THC consumed.
Results
Six subjects consumed one cigarette, thirteen smoked two and four smoked three (data
from one male subject were excluded from the results because no drug was found in his
plasma after smoking). The average amount of THC consumed was 20.8 mg, after
adjustment for body weight, 308 mg/kg. It should be noted that these amounts of THC
represent both the inhaled dose and the portion that was lost through pyrolysis and side-
stream smoke during the smoking process. There were no significant differences
between males and females, nor between frequent and infrequent users, with respect to
the weight adjusted preferred dose. It was decided that the maximum dose for
subsequent driving studies would be 300 mg/kg.
Study 1: Driving on a Restricted Highway
Methods
The first driving study was conducted on a highway closed to other traffic. The same
twelve men and twelve women who participated in the laboratory study served again as
the subjects. They were treated on separate occasions with marijuana cigarettes
containing THC doses of 0 (placebo), 100, 200, and 300 mg/kg. Treatments were
administered double-blind and in a counterbalanced order. On each occasion, subjects
performed a road-tracking test beginning 40 minutes after initiation of smoking and
repeated one hour later. The test involved maintaining a constant speed at 90 km/h and
a steady lateral position between the delineated boundaries of the traffic lane. Subjects
drove 22 km on a primary highway and were accompanied by a licensed driving
instructor. The primary dependent variable was the standard deviation of lateral position
(SDLP), which has been shown to be both highly reliable and very sensitive to the
influence of sedative medicinal drugs and alcohol. Other dependent variables were
mean speed, and standard deviations of speed and steering wheel angle. Blood
samples were taken 10 minutes before the driving tests (i.e. 30 and 90 minutes after
initiation of smoking, respectively).
Results
All subjects were willing and
able to finish the driving tests
without great difficulty. Data
from one male subject were
excluded from the results
because no drug was found
in his plasma after smoking.
marijuana impairs driving
performance as measured by
an increase in lateral position
variability: all three THC
doses significantly affected
SDLP relative to placebo
(p<.012, .001 & .001, for the
100, 200 & 300 mg/kg
conditions, respectively. The
Dose by Time effect was not Figure Mean (+SE) SDLP by THC dose and time.
significant indicating that
impairment after marijuana was the same in both trials. Marijuana's effects on SDLP were
compared to those of alcohol obtained in a very similar study by Louwerens et al. (1987).
It appeared that the effects of the various administered THC doses (100-300 mg/kg) on
SDLP were equivalent to those associated with BACs in the range of 0.03-0.07 g%. Other
driving performance measures were not significantly affected by THC. Plasma
concentrations of the drug were clearly related to the administered dose and time of
blood sampling but unrelated to driving performance impairment.
Study 2: Driving on a Normal Highway in Traffic
Methods
The second driving study was conducted on a highway in the presence of other traffic
and involved both a road-tracking and a car-following test. A new group of sixteen
subjects, equally comprised of men and women, participated in this study. A
conservative approach was chosen in designing the present study in order to satisfy the
strictest safety requirements. That is, the study was conducted according to an
ascending dose series design where both active drug and placebo conditions were
administered, double-blind, at each of three THC dose levels. THC doses were the same
as those used in the previous study, namely 100, 200, and 300 mg/kg. Cigarettes
appeared identical at each level of treatment conditions. If any subject would have
reacted in an unacceptable manner to a lower dose, he/she would not have been
permitted to receive a higher dose.
The subjects began the car-following test 45 minutes after smoking. The test was performed on a 16 km segment of the highway and lasted about 15 minutes. After theconclusion of this test, subjects performed a 64-km road-tracking test on the samehighway which lasted about 50 minutes. At the conclusion of this test, they participatedagain in the car-following test. Blood samples were taken both before the first and afterthe last driving test (i.e. 35 and 190 minutes after initiation of smoking, respectively).
The road-tracking test was the same as in the previous study except for its duration and the presence of other traffic. The car-following test involved attempting to matchvelocity with, and maintain a constant distance from a preceding vehicle as it executed aseries of deceleration/acceleration maneuvers. The preceding vehicle's speed wouldvary between 80 and 100 km/h and the subject was instructed to maintain a 50 mdistance however the preceding vehicle's speed might vary. The duration of onedeceleration and acceleration maneuver was approximately 50 seconds and six to eightof these maneuvers were executed during one test, depending upon traffic density. Thesubject's average reaction time to the movements of the preceding vehicle, meandistance and coefficient of variation of distance during maneuvers were taken as thedependent variables from this.
Results
All subjects were able to
complete the series without
suffering any untoward
reaction while driving. Data
from one female subject
were excluded from the
results because no drug was
found in her plasma after
smoking.
performance in the standard
test was impaired in a dose-
related manner by THC and
confirmed the results
obtained in the previous
closed highway study (Fig.
2). The 100 mg/kg dose
produced a slight elevation in
mean SDLP, albeit not Figure Mean (+SED) changes in SDLP in the standard
statistically significant driving test by THC dose, relative to placebo.
(p<.13). The 200 mg/kg dose
produced a significant (p<.023) elevation, of dubious practical relevance. The 300 mg/kg
dose produced a highly significant (p<.007) elevation which may be viewed as practically
relevant. After marijuana smoking, subjects drove with an average speed that was only
slightly lower than after placebo and very close to the prescribed level.
In the car-following test, subjects maintained a distance of 45-50 m while driving in the successive placebo conditions. They lengthened mean distance by 8, 6 and 2 m inthe corresponding THC conditions after 100, 200 and 300 mg/kg, respectively. Theinitially large drug-placebo difference and its subsequent decline is a surprising result.
Our explanation for this observation is that the subjects' caution was greatest the firsttime they undertook the test under the influence of THC and progressively less thereafter.
The reaction time of the subjects to changes in the preceding vehicle's speed increasedfollowing THC treatment, relative to placebo. The administered THC dose was inverselyrelated to the change in reaction time, as it was to distance. However, increased reactiontimes were partly due to longer distance (i.e. the longer the distance to the precedingvehicle, the more difficult it is to perceive changes in its speed). Statistical adjustment for this confounding variable resulted in smaller and non-significant increases in reactiontime following marijuana treatment, the greatest impairment (0.32 s) being observed inthe first test following the lowest THC dose (Fig. 3). Distance variability followed a similarpattern as mean distance and reaction time; the greatest impairment was found followingthe lowest dose. As in the previous study, plasma concentrations of the drug were notrelated to driving impairment.
Study 3: Driving in Urban
Traffic

Methods
The program proceeded into
the third driving study, which
involved tests conducted in
high-density urban traffic.
There were logical and
safety reasons for restricting
the THC dose to 100 mg/kg. It
was given to a new group of
16 regular marijuana (or
hashish) users, along with a
placebo. For comparative
purposes, another group of
16 regular users of alcohol,
but not marijuana, were
treated with a modest dose
of their preferred recreational Figure Mean (+SED) changes in `adjusted' reaction
drug, ethanol, and again time by THC dose and time, relative to placebo.
placebo, before undertaking
the same city driving test. Both groups were equally comprised of men and women.
Marijuana was administered to deliver 100 mg/kg THC. The driving test commenced 30minutes after smoking. The alcohol dose was chosen to yield a BAC approaching0.05 g% when the driving test commenced 45 minutes after onset of drinking. Activedrug and placebo conditions were administered double-blind and in a counterbalancedorder in each group. Blood samples were taken immediately prior to and following allplacebo and drug driving tests (i.e. 20 and 80 minutes after initiation of smoking, or 35and 95 minutes after initiation of drinking).
Driving tests were conducted in daylight over a constant 17.5 km route within the city limits of Maastricht. Subjects drove their placebo and active-drug rides through heavy,medium and low density traffic on the same day of the week, and at the same time ofday. Two scoring methods were employed in the present study. The first, a `molecular'approach adopted from Jones (1978), involved the employment of a specially trainedobserver who applied simple and strict criteria for recording when the driver made orfailed to make each in a series of observable responses at predetermined points along achosen route. The second, a `molar' approach, required the driving instructor acting asthe safety controller during the tests to retrospectively rate the driver's performanceusing a shortened version of the Royal Dutch Tourist Association's Driving Proficiency Test. In total, 108 items were dichotomously scored, as either pass or fail. Total testperformance was measured by the percentage items scored as `pass'. Subscores werecalculated for vehicle checks, vehicle handling, traffic maneuvers, observation andunderstanding of traffic, and turning'. This method has been applied previously to showthe impairing effects of alcohol and diazepam (De Gier, 1979; De Gier et al., 1981).
Results
Data from two male subjects in the marijuana group were excluded from the results
because neither THC nor THC-COOH was found in their plasma after smoking.
Neither alcohol nor marijuana significantly affected driving performance measures obtained by the molecular approach, indicating that it may be relatively insensitive todrug-induced changes. The molar approach was more sensitive. Table 1 shows that amodest dose of alcohol (BAC=0.034 g%) produced a significant impairment in city driving,relative to placebo. More specifically, alcohol impaired both vehicle handling and trafficmaneuvers. Marijuana, administered in a dose of 100 mg/kg THC, on the other hand, didnot significantly change mean driving performance as measured by this approach.
Subjects' ratings of driving quality and effort to accomplish the task were strikinglydifferent from the driving instructor's ratings. Both groups rated their driving performancefollowing placebo as somewhat better than `normal'. Following the active drug, ratingswere significantly lower (35%, p<.009) in the marijuana, but not (5%, ns) in the alcoholgroup. Perceived effort to accomplish the driving test was about the same in both groupsfollowing placebo. Following the active drug, a significant (p<.033) increase in perceivedeffort was reported by the marijuana, but not the alcohol group.
Table 1 Mean (±SED) changes in driving performance scores measured by the molar
approach for the marijuana (N=14) and alcohol (N=16) group; and, the significance of
each change and difference between changes.
Thus, there is evidence that subjects in the marijuana group were not only aware of their intoxicated condition, but were also attempting to compensate for it. These seem tobe important findings. They support both the common belief that drivers becomeoverconfident after drinking alcohol and investigators' suspicions that they become morecautious and self-critical after consuming low doses of THC, as smoked marijuana.
Drug plasma concentrations were neither related to absolute driving performance scores nor to the changes that occurred from placebo to drug conditions. With respect toTHC, these results confirm the findings in previous studies. They are somewhatsurprising for alcohol but may be due to the restricted range of ethanol concentrations inthe plasma of different subjects.
Discussion
The results of the studies corroborate those of previous driving simulator and closed-course tests by indicating that THC in inhaled doses up to 300 mg/kg has significant, yetnot dramatic, dose-related impairing effects on driving performance (cf. Smiley, 1986).
Standard deviation of lateral position in the road-tracking test was the most sensitivemeasure for revealing THC's adverse effects. This is because road-tracking is primarilycontrolled by an automatic information processing system which operates outside ofconscious control. The process is relatively impervious to environmental changes buthighly vulnerable to internal factors that retard the flow of information through thesystem. THC and many other drugs are among these factors. When they interfere withthe process that restricts road-tracking error, there is little the afflicted individual can doby way of compensation to restore the situation. Car-following and, to a greater extent,city driving performance depend more on controlled information processing and aretherefore more accessible for compensatory mechanisms that reduce the decrements orabolish them entirely.
THC's effects on road-tracking after doses up to 300 mg/kg never exceeded alcohol's at BACs of 0.08 g%; and, were in no way unusual compared to many medicinal drugs'(Robbe, 1994; Robbe and O'Hanlon, 1995; O'Hanlon et al., 1995). Yet, THC's effectsdiffer qualitatively from many other drugs, especially alcohol. Evidence from the presentand previous studies strongly suggests that alcohol encourages risky driving whereasTHC encourages greater caution, at least in experiments. Another way THC seems todiffer qualitatively from many other drugs is that the former's users seem better able tocompensate for its adverse effects while driving under the influence.
Inter-subject correlations between plasma concentrations of the drug and driving performance after every dose were essentially nil, partly due to the peculiar kinetics ofTHC. It enters the brain relatively rapidly, although with a perceptible delay relative toplasma concentrations. Once there, it remains even at a time when plasmaconcentrations approach or reach zero. As a result, performance may still be impaired atthe time that plasma concentrations of the drug are near the detection limit. This isexactly what happened in the first driving study. Therefore an important practicalimplications of the study is that is not possible to conclude anything about a driver'simpairment on the basis of his/her plasma concentrations of THC and THC-COOHdetermined in a single sample.
Although THC's adverse effects on driving performance appeared relatively small in thetests employed in this program, one can still easily imagine situations where theinfluence of marijuana smoking might have a dangerous effect; i.e., emergency situations which put high demands on the driver's information processing capacity,prolonged monotonous driving, and after THC has been taken with other drugs,especially alcohol. Because these possibilities are real, the results of the present studiesshould not be considered as the final word. They should, however, serve as the point ofdeparture for subsequent studies that will ultimately complete the picture of THC's effectson driving performance.
References
De Gier JJ (1979) A subjective measurement of the influence of ethyl/alcohol in moderate doses on real driving performances. Blutalkohol, 16, 363–370.
De Gier JJ, 't Hart BJ, Nelemans FA and Bergman H (1981) Psychomotor performance and real driving performance of outpatients receiving diazepam.
Psychopharmacology, 73, 340–347.
Jones MH (1978) Driver Performance Measures for the Safe Performance Curriculum. Traffic Safety Center, Institute of Safety and Systems Management,University of South California, Los Angeles, CA (DOT HS 803 461).
Louwerens JW, Gloerich ABM, de Vries G, Brookhuis KA and O'Hanlon JF (1987).
The relationship between drivers' blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actualdriving performance during high speed travel. Pages 183-192 in PC Noordzij andR Roszbach, eds., Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety. Proceedings of the 10thInternational Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety. Excerpta Medica,Amsterdam.
O'Hanlon JF, Vermeeren A, Uiterwijk MMC, van Veggel LMA and Swijgman HF (1995) Anxiolytics' effects on the actual driving performance of patients andhealthy volunteers in a standardized test: an integration of three studies.
Neuropsychobiology, 31:81-88.
Robbe HWJ (1994). Influence of Marijuana on Driving. PhD thesis, Institute for Human Psychopharmacology, University of Limburg, Maastricht.
Robbe HWJ and O'Hanlon JF (1995) Acute and subchronic effects of paroxetine and amitriptyline on actual driving, psychomotor performance and subjectiveassessments in healthy volunteers. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 5:35-42.
Smiley AM (1986). Marijuana: On-road and driving simulator studies. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving: Abstracts and Reviews 2: 121-134.

Source: http://www.bisdro.uni-bremen.de/boellinger/cannabis/09-robbe.pdf

Office use only

OFFICE USE ONLY DATE TO START CLASS NAME DAYS REQUIRED Little Academy Nursery Application Form Child’s Information Family Information Current Residental address Emergency Contact Information Attendance Information My child will attend: □ Full-Time □ 3 days (Sun, Tues, Thurs) □ 2 days (Mon, Wed) Has your child been in a nurser

Microsoft word - timetable year 4 sem i _2012-2013_

TIMETABLE FOR YEAR 4 SEMESTER I, 2012-2013 FACULTY OF PHARMACY FACULTY OF PHARMACY UNIVERSITI KEBANGSAAN MALAYSIA YEAR 4, SEMESTER I, 2012-2013 CODE COURSE COORDINATOR NFNF4095 7964 adliah@pharmacy.ukm.my NFNF4235 6232 sitiazdiah@yahoo.com NFNF4433 7972 mazlina.said@pharmacy.ukm.my NFNF4014 7974 nsfern@pharmacy.ukm.my NFNF4312 73

Copyright © 2010-2014 Metabolize Drugs Pdf